Tải bản đầy đủ (.pdf) (8 trang)

_Ha Vu Kien - 2012 - A Meta-analysis of the consequences of virtualness on team functioning

Bạn đang xem bản rút gọn của tài liệu. Xem và tải ngay bản đầy đủ của tài liệu tại đây (328.85 KB, 8 trang )

<span class='text_page_counter'>(1)</span><div class='page_container' data-page=1>

A

meta-analysis

of

the

consequences

of

virtualness

on

team

functioning



Ana

Ortiz

de

Guinea

a,1

,

Jane

Webster

b,

*

,

D.

Sandy

Staples

b,2


a<sub>HEC</sub><sub>Montre´al,</sub><sub>3000,</sub><sub>chemin</sub><sub>de</sub><sub>la</sub><sub>Coˆte-Sainte-Catherine,</sub><sub>Montre´al,</sub><sub>Que´bec</sub><sub>H3T</sub><sub>2A7,</sub><sub>Canada</sub>
b<sub>Queen’s</sub><sub>School</sub><sub>of</sub><sub>Business,</sub><sub>143</sub><sub>Union</sub><sub>Street,</sub><sub>Kingston,</sub><sub>Ontario</sub><sub>K7L</sub><sub>3N6,</sub><sub>Canada</sub>


1. Introduction


Thepracticeofusingvirtualteamsinorganizationshasbecome


popular. Thus, it is important to organizations and team


researcherstounderstandhow‘‘virtualness’’affectsteam


perfor-mance.Wedecidedtoreviewtheempiricalresearchonthistopic


throughameta-analysis.


Virtualteamshavebeendefinedasgroupsofindividualswho


work together in different locations on interdependent tasks,


sharing the responsibility for outcomes, while relying on


technologytoprovidemostoftheircommunication.Whileearly


virtual team research usually examined ‘‘virtualness’’ as a


dichotomy, either face-to-face or computer-mediated (without



physicalinteraction),virtualnesshasevolvedtoincludedegreeof


separation of members(distance), proportionof memberswho


workvirtually (configuration),and theproportion of time that


teammembersworkapart.WeadoptedSchweitzerandDuxbury’s


[19]suggestionthat‘‘tobeconsideredvirtual,ateammusthave


somememberswhodonotworkineitherthesameplaceand/orat


thesametime,andtherefore,cannotcollaborateface-to-faceallof


the time’’. Thus, we focused on the degree of virtualness, a


continuum,rangingfromnotatallvirtualtohighlyvirtual.We


onlyincludedarticlesinourstudythatmeasuredorvariedoneor


moredimensionsofvirtualness.3 <sub>In</sub><sub>doing</sub><sub>so,</sub> <sub>this</sub><sub>article</sub><sub>differs</sub>


fromothersthatincludedarticlesthatdonotassessorvarythe


level ofvirtualness[16]orthatdo notanalyzetherelationship


betweenvirtualnessandothervariables[10].


Narrativereviewsoftheliteraturehavehighlighted



inconsis-tent findings, such as positive and negative [e.g., 7] relations


between virtualnessandperformance.Understandingwhysuch


inconsistencies exist could help practitioners. The narrative


reviewshave alsopointedout a predominanceof certaintypes


ofstudies,suchasshort-termexperimentalresearch,andquestion


whether theresults are generalizable. In contrast, quantitative


reviews, suchasmeta-analyses,helptoaddressthese


inconsis-tencyissuesandquestions.


Meta-analyses quantitatively integrate results reported in


existing studies. The analysis increases the power of the


conclusions made on the relations between variables. Unlike


narrativereviews, meta-analysesadjust forsamplesizeandthe


reliability of measures, thus providing better estimates of the


ARTICLE INFO


Articlehistory:
Received8July2010



Receivedinrevisedform6June2012
Accepted21August2012
Availableonline25September2012


Keywords:
Meta-analysis
Distributed
Virtualness
Group
Performance
Knowledgesharing
Satisfaction
Conflict
Time


ABSTRACT


Virtualteamsinorganizations havenowbecomeareality,but therehave beenonlyahandfulof


quantitativereviewson‘‘virtualness’’(i.e.,teamsthataremoreorlessvirtual).Wedecidedtoconducta


meta-analytic review of the effects of virtualness on team functioning (conflict, communication


frequency,knowledgesharing,performance,andsatisfaction).Toexplaininconsistenciesintheresults


of publishedmaterial on thetopic, we also examinedthe moderatingeffects of levelofanalysis


(individual/group),method (experiment/survey), andtime frame (short/long). Eightystudies were



foundthatcoveredsomepartofthisdomain.Resultsseemtodifferintherelativeimportanceofthe


factors.Thusthoughaggregatedfindingssuggestednegativeeffectsofvirtualnessonteamfunctioning,


resultsvaried in strength and direction of themoderators, indicating thatit was not possibleto


generalize.Forexample,thenegativeeffectsheldonlyforshort-termteams,whileinlonger-termteams


theeffectsweakenedordisappeared.


ß2012ElsevierB.V.Allrightsreserved.


*Correspondingauthor.Tel.:+16135333163;fax:+16135332325.
E-mailaddresses:(A.OrtizdeGuinea),


(J.Webster),


(D.S.Staples).


1


Tel:+15143407817;fax:+15143406132.


2


Tel:+16135332314;fax:+16135332325.


3<sub>We</sub><sub>excluded</sub><sub>articles</sub><sub>that</sub><sub>did</sub><sub>not</sub><sub>have</sub><sub>either</sub><sub>a</sub><sub>measure</sub><sub>of</sub><sub>virtualness</sub><sub>or</sub><sub>a</sub>


comparisonofvirtualwithface-to-faceteams.Neitherdidwequantitativelyreview


articlesatotherlevelsofanalysis,suchasvirtualcommunitiesororganizations,as
thiswouldconfoundtheresults.


ContentslistsavailableatSciVerseScienceDirect


Information

&

Management



j our na l ho me pa ge : w ww . e l se v i e r . com / l oca t e / i m


0378-7206/$–seefrontmatterß2012ElsevierB.V.Allrightsreserved.


</div>
<span class='text_page_counter'>(2)</span><div class='page_container' data-page=2>

relationsbetweenvariables.Theyalsoallowforthediscoveryof


moderatorsthrough thecoding ofdifferences between studies.


Thus,progress intheorybuildingand cumulative knowledgeis


achievablewithmeta-analysis.


Theobjectivesofourmeta-analysiswereto:(1)examinethe


extent to which virtual teams research has built cumulative


knowledge,(2)quantifythestrengthoftherelationshipsbetween


virtualnessand team processes and outcomes, and (3) explore


reasonsforcontradictoryfindings.


2. Theconsequencesofvirtualness



Toexaminetheeffectsofvirtualnessinteamwork,weusedthe


well-knowninput–process–output(IPO)modelthatincludesinput


factorssuchasteammembercharacteristics,team-and


organiza-tional-levelarchitecture,contextualinformation,andvirtualness;


processeswhicharetheinteractionsbetweenteam4membersor


communicationsfromindividualsinvolvingteamwork,thatmay


beeitherexpressive(interpersonal)orinstrumental(task-related);


andoutputsthatmeasureteameffectiveness,suchasperformance


onthetaskorthesatisfactionofgroupmembers.


The IPOmodel hasbeenmodifiedas morehasbeenlearned


aboutteameffectiveness; forexample,somemediating


mecha-nisms, initially considered processes, have been identified as


emergentstates(e.g.,cognitive,motivational, oraffectivestates


such as potency and cohesion). Therefore, the general term


mediatorisusedtodescribeprocessesandemergentstates.Time



hasalsobeenrecognizedtohaveanimportantrole,whichwasnot


capturedwellintheIPOmodel.ThesechangeshaveledtoanIMOI


(input–mediator–output–input)framework[14].Withinthisnew


framework,itis possibletoexplaintheeffectsofvirtualnesson


mediatorsand/oroutcomeswiththetheoreticalperspectives of


mediarichness/socialpresence,attributiontheory,and


categori-zation.


Today,groupsaremorelikelytobedispersed,withelectronic


communicationdominatingtheirinteraction.Mediarichnessand


socialpresencetheoriessuggestthatsuchcommunicationisless


personal, with fewer nonverbal cues. Also, attribution theory


suggeststhatpeopletrytoexplaintheirownorothers’behaviors


bymakingtwotypesofcasualattributions:internal(dispositional)


orexternal(situational).Aperson’sinitialattributionofanother’s


behaviorisusuallydispositional(i.e.,thefundamentalattribution



error)andthencorrected,dependingonknowledgeoftheperson


and/orsituation.Virtualteamsarelikelytomakeattributionerrors


becausemembers haveless knowledgeoftheir teammatesand


theirenvironments.Thusthereispotentialforattributionerrorsto


go uncorrected [6]. Categorization provides a third theoretical


perspective:socialidentitytheory,self-categorizationtheoryand


thesimilarity/attractionparadigmallsuggestthatpeople


catego-rize themselves into subgroups according to salient cues.


Individualsidentify more closely with people they perceive as


beingsimilartothemselves[5].Invirtualteamsettings,subgroups


maydevelop.Asin-andout-groupcharacteristicsbecomesalient


insubgroups,individualsbecomemorebiasedtowardstheirown


subgroups[20].


3. Developmentofresearchquestionsandhypotheses


Virtualnessistheonlyinputvariableweexamined.Considering



thenumberofmediatorandoutcomeconstructs,thenumberof


possiblerelationshipsisquitelarge.Wesearchedtheliteraturefor


empiricalstudiesrelatedtovirtualteams.Whenevervirtualness


wasincluded in a study and its relationship to a mediator or


outcomevariablehadbeenexaminedinatleastoneotherstudy,


themediator/outputvariablewasincludedinourmeta-analysis.


Therefore,ourfirstgeneralresearchquestionwas:


Question 1: What are the strengths and directions of the


relationships between virtualness and team mediators and


outputs?


We also examined the generalizability of the findings.


Specifically, by introducingmoderatingvariables, meta-analysis


techniques can determine whether between-study differences


werepartiallydue tothedifferentconditionsin themoderator


variables.Researchdesignsandsamplecharacteristicsaretypical



moderatorsexaminedinmeta-analysiswork. Accordingly,three


possible moderators that varied most frequently across the


primarystudieswereinvestigated:thelevelofanalysis(individual


versusgroup),thestudymethod(experimentversussurvey),and


the time theteams worked together (short versus long-term).


Therefore,oursecondresearchquestionreferredtomoderators:


Question2:Towhatextentdolevelofanalysis,studymethod,


and/or time duration moderate the relationships between


virtualnessandteamprocessesandoutputs?


Toinvestigatethesequestions,wedevelopedspecific


hypothe-sesabouttheeffectsofvirtualnessontheselectedmediatorsand


outcomes.Thechoiceofprocessesandoutcomestoincludeinour


meta-analysiswasdrivenbythreefactors.First,weneededtypical


processes and outcomes to be represented. Second, we were


restrictedtosituationsinwhichmultipleempiricalstudiesonthe



relationshipbetweenvirtualnessanda specificvariableexisted.


Third,wewishedtofocusonvariableswheretheempiricalresults


wereambiguous,sothatourmeta-analysiswouldhelptoclarify


them. Three mediating variables (conflict, communication


fre-quency, knowledge sharing) and two output variables


(perfor-mance,andsatisfaction)metourcriteria.


3.1. Maineffecthypotheses


Conflict represents perceived discrepancies, incompatible


desires, and wishes of the parties involved in a team. Conflict


maybedividedintothreetypes:relationshipconflict(theaffective


componentofconflictthatconcernstheawarenessof


incompati-bilities intheinterpersonal realm);taskconflict (differencesin


opinionsaboutagrouptask);andprocessconflict(differencesof


opinionaboutthewayataskshouldbeperformed).Researchers


have usedconflict measureswithlabels suchas: proportion of



disagreement, personal attacks, tension, task conflict, process


conflict,relationshipconflict,affectiveconflictandgeneralconflict,


oracombinationofthese.Allthreetypesrelatetovirtualteam


functioning[9].Forexample,itcanbearguedthatmoderatetask


conflictcanbebeneficialbecauseitresultsindiscussionthatcan


provideabettersolutiontosolvethetask.Indeed,Masseyetal.


[13]foundthatmoreproductivevirtualteamsexperiencedmore


conflict.However,manystudiesfindthatthethreetypesofconflict


inter-relatestronglybutnegativelytoteamfunctioning[e.g.,8].


Wethereforeexpectedthatthoseinmorevirtualteamswill


experiencemoreconflict,whilethoseinlessvirtualteamsenjoy


more face-to-face communication, resulting in more informal


interaction and socialization. This is likely to result in greater


rapport and collaboration, stronger team identity, and lower


conflict.Becauseofdistancebetweenteammembers,conflictis



difficult tomanage in morevirtual teams. Differences in work


locationcanalsoleadtotheformationofin-groupsandout-groups


withinteams.Thus,conflicttypicallyarisesbetweenthesubgroups


duetofavoritismoflocalmembers.


Althoughmoststudiesfoundpositiverelations(higherconflict


for more virtual teams), a few have demonstrated a negative


4


</div>
<span class='text_page_counter'>(3)</span><div class='page_container' data-page=3>

relationship [e.g.,21]. Nevertheless, given that the majority of


empiricalresearchshowsapositiverelationbetweenvirtualness


andconflict,weexpectedthatmorevirtualteamswillexperience


moreconflict:


Hypothesis1:Virtualnessrelatespositivelytoconflict.


Our secondhypothesis concerns theeffectof virtualness on


communication frequency (the amount of communication


be-tweenteammembers). Withoutcommunication,lackofmutual



knowledge,misunderstandings(attributionerrors),andalackof


contextualknowledgemayoccur.Unfortunately,communication


representsa challengeforvirtualteams.Althoughthosein less


virtualteamsmayusethesamecommunicationtechnologiesand


communicate at the same frequency, patterns of face-to-face


communicationcan differ. Co-locationpromotesinformal


com-municationandthereforemoreoverallcommunication.


Mostempiricalresearchsupportsthis,forexample,O’Learyand


Cummings [17] found that communication frequency related


negatively to virtualness. Thus, we expected that more virtual


teamswouldcommunicatelessandhypothesized:


Hypothesis2:Virtualnessrelatesnegativelytocommunication


frequency.


Knowledgesharingrepresentsanotherimportantteamprocess.


Researchershaveusedmeasuressuchasamountofinformation



shared, transferred, or exchanged within teams. In traditional


teams,thesharingofexpertiseisanessentialgroupprocessfor


teameffectiveness.Sharingofinformationandknowledgeiseven


more critical for cross-functional teamwork and good decision


making[18].


Few studies have examined the effect of virtualness on


knowledge sharing, and within those, the results have been


somewhat mixed,though most reported negative relationships


[e.g.,1].Itis,ofcourse,moredifficulttosharerichinformationand


knowledge through electronic media than face-to-face. Also,


geographic dispersion may lower employees’ attention to the


virtualteamtask.Geographicdiversitycanalsoleadtothecreation


ofsub-groupswithinateam,wherein-groupsfavorlocalmembers


andsharelessknowledgeoverall.Teamidentitycanalsobehigher


inlessvirtualgroupsduetomoreinteractionandsocialpresence.



Finally,becausevirtualteamsareoftenculturallydiverse,language


skillsmightlimitknowledgesharing.Thus:


Hypothesis 3: Virtualness relates negatively to knowledge


sharing.


Outcomes represent the results and by-products that have


valuetotheorganization; theymaybesplitinthreeclasses(or


dimensions):performance,which relates totheteam’staskand


includesteam efficiency, productivity,and innovation; affective


reactions, which include satisfaction and commitment; and


behaviors,whichincludeindicatorsofthegroup’sabilitytoexist


overtimesuchasturnoverandabsenteeism.


We focused on two outcomes: team performance and


satisfaction. Performance represents the effectiveness of the


outcome with respect to the specific task or project at hand.


Researchershaveincludedmeasuressuchasproject outcomes



(e.g., met requirements, within budget, within schedule) and


confidenceinthedecisionreachedbythegroup.Becausemuchof


thecommunicationsinvirtualsettingstakeplaceelectronically,


causal factors, such as lower social presence and slower


communication, might affect performance. Asynchronous and


slower-pacedcommunicationmayleadtolessfocusandattention


totheteamactivity,resultinginlower performance.Invirtual


teams,trusttakeslongertodevelopandcanbemorefragilethan


inface-to-faceteams.Thus,lowertrustcannegativelyinfluence


teamperformance.Developingmutualknowledgeabout


team-mates’environmentsisalsochallengingwhenthereisgeographic


separation,andthelackofthisknowledgecanleadtoattribution


errorsandundermineteameffectiveness.


Nonetheless,virtualnesscanalsoprovidebenefits.Forexample,


creativity of the team may be enhanced by bringing diverse



perspectives to bear and encouraging people to share their


opinions. Lessproduction blockingtakesplace inasynchronous


communicationsincepeoplecanworkatthesametime,aswellas


rehearsetheircommunications.However,mostempiricalarticles


suggest anegativerelationshipbetweenvirtualnessand


perfor-mance.Therefore:


Hypothesis 4: Virtualnessrelates negatively toteam


perfor-mance.


Thereareseveralwaysinwhichvirtualnesscouldaffectteam


satisfaction. In virtual settings, relationships take longer to


develop.Strongerinterpersonalrelationshipsandteamtieshave


been linkedtomotivation and less process loss,which in turn


affects team satisfaction. More developed teams communicate


more constructively, have more mutual understanding and


knowledge (i.e., fewer attribution errors), are able to manage



conflictmoreeffectively,andaremorecohesive.Thereforevirtual


teams may have lower satisfaction, at least in theshort-term.


Cyber-ostracism, where one ormore individualsis excluded in


groupinteractions,canalsonegativelyimpactteamsatisfactionin


virtualteams.Therefore:


Hypothesis5:Virtualnessrelatesnegativelytoteam


satisfac-tion.


3.2. Moderatinghypotheses


Someofthereportedresearchfindingsaremixed;forexample,


Martinsetal.[12]notethemixed(positiveandnegative)relations


between virtualness and team functioning. The examination of


moderatorsmighthelptoshedlightontheseconflictingresults.


Therefore,weconsideredhowourmoderators(levelofanalysis,


studymethod,andteamduration)couldalterourfivehypotheses.


Resultsatonelevelofanalysismaynotgeneralizetoanother



andthusmixinglevelsofanalysismayintroduceambiguitiesinto


theresults[2].Teamshavecharacteristicsthataredifferentfrom


thatofindividualswhomaynotagreewiththeperceptionoftheir


team-mates.Further, resultsmaybestronger attheteam level


becauseconstructsusuallycapturesharedbeliefs.Therefore:


Hypothesis6:Thelevelofanalysis(groupversusindividual)


moderatesthevirtualnessrelationships.Morespecifically,for


groups, rather than for individuals, virtualness relates more


positivelyto(i)conflictandmorenegativelyto(ii)


communi-cation frequency, (iii) knowledge sharing, (iv) team


perfor-mance,and(v)satisfaction.


Most experiments are made on ad hoc, temporary groups,


whosemembersareparticipatingforcoursecreditandarelocated


inoneuniversity;incontrast,mostsurveysaremadeon


longer-term groups of employees from multiple organizations and



countries. Becauseparticipantsinexperimentsgenerallyhavea


specifictasktocompletewithinashorttimeframe,allparticipants


(generally students) communicate to the extent necessary,


(throughwhatever mediaareavailable) tocompletetheir task.


Thus,experimentalparticipantstendtobededicatedtothetask


andtheeffectsofdistanceareminimized.Thiswouldsuggestthat


weakerrelationshipsforvirtualnessoccurinexperimentsthanin


surveys. However, one could also take the opposite view and


</div>
<span class='text_page_counter'>(4)</span><div class='page_container' data-page=4>

experimentstobeshorter-term(andthelogicof H8,presented


next),wemightexpectteamprocessesandoutcomestobeless


positiveforexperiments.Aswecannotpredictthedirectionofthe


moderation,weposit:


Hypothesis 7: The study methodmoderates thevirtualness


relationships. Morespecifically,virtualnesswillrelate


differ-ently to (i) conflict, (ii) communication frequency, (iii)



knowledgesharing,(iv)teamperformance,and(v)satisfaction


forsurveysandexperiments.


Thethirdmoderatorwastheamountoftimethatteamsworked


together.Thisisimportantbecauselonger-termteamsappearto


bemorelikelytoexistinactualorganizationalsettings,andteam


processesandcommitmentchangeovertime.Thenegativeeffects


ofdiversityarelikelytobeneutralizedasmembersspendmore


timetogether[15].Similarly,communicationpatternscanchange,


starting with unidirectional communication but ending with


mutualcommunication.Therefore,asteamsdevelopmorefully,


differencesbetween face-to-face and distributed teamstend to


disappear,and teammembersbecomemorewillingtoworkto


understandtheproblemsofotherteammembersovertime[3].


Thus,team duration canchangetheamountof timethat team


memberswanttointeractandtheperceivedbenefitsofinvesting



insocialandworkingrelationships.Therefore:


Hypothesis 8: Team duration moderates the virtualness


relationships. More specifically,forshorter termrather than


longer termteams,virtualness relates morepositively to(i)


conflictandmorenegativelyto(ii)communicationfrequency,


(iii) knowledge sharing, (iv) team performance, and (v)


satisfaction.


4. Method


By using meta-analysis, the strengths of the relationships


betweentwoormoreconstructscanbeassessedbyquantitatively


combining resultsfrom existing (primary) empirical studies to


determine the overall relationships. Inconsistent findings are


commonamongprimarystudiesduetomeasurementerror,low


statisticalpower, anddifferentresearchcontexts.Meta-analytic


techniquescanhelpovercomesomeoftheseproblems.



Weusedseveralwaystofindempiricalstudiesandupdatedthe


datawithmorerecentsearches.Articleswerenotrestrictedtoany


discipline and were found by searching databases (PsycINFO,


ProQuest,WebofScience)andconferenceproceedings(Academyof


Management (AOM), Americas Conference on Information


Sys-tems, Computer Supported Cooperative Work, International


Conferenceon Information Systems), obtaining workingpapers


throughpersonalcontacts,searchingtwo Websitesfocused on


virtual team research (VoNet and virtualteamresearch.org),


posting calls for unpublished and ‘in press’ papers (AISWorld,


AOM’sOrganizational CommunicationandInformation Systems


division),reviewingcollections of articleson virtual teamwork,


conducting a general Google Web search, and examining


referencesinreviewedarticlestoidentifynewleads.Thisresulted


inasubstantialvolumeofarticlesbeingretrieved;forexample,



ProQuestcoversapproximately1800businessjournalsfrom1971


tothepresent time.Our searchterms were:virtual, virtualness,


distributed, dispersed, global, or remote combined with team or


group.Weexcludedstudiesthatcontainedonlyqualitativedata,


examinedarelationshipthatwasnotusedbyanyotherauthor


and/ordidnotcontainallofthedataweneeded5<sub>.</sub><sub>Although</sub><sub>the</sub>


searches provided us with almost 400 articles that might be


includedinourmeta-analysis,manyhadtobeexcludedbecause


they did not match our criteria. For example, for one of our


searches(aftertheremovalofqualitativeandtheoreticalarticles),


62%didnotcapturevirtualness(eitherasameasureofvirtualness


orasacomparisonbetweenvirtualandface-to-faceteams),30%


didnotcontainthequantitativedataneeded,6%didnotinclude


enoughinformation for theirinclusion, and 2%werearticlesin


which the data had already been published in another article



alreadyinourmeta-analysis.Fromalloursearches,wefound80


uniqueandusefuldatasetsfromthe79articleslistedintheonline


Appendix(see />


4.1. Codingthedata


Meta-analysiscanbeusedtosummarizerelationshipsbetween


variables (effect sizes) using correlations, mean differences, or


proportions.Manymeta-analystssummarizecorrelationsbecause


oftheirprevalenceinresearcharticles,whichiswhatwedid.For


our analysis, one author coded all of the papers based on


discussions with the other two authors. Once the coding was


complete,wehiredagraduatestudentwhowasblindtoourcoding


toensurethatthedirectionoftheeffectswasappropriatelycoded


foreachstudy6<sub>.</sub><sub>Any</sub><sub>discrepancies</sub><sub>were</sub><sub>resolved</sub><sub>by</sub><sub>the</sub><sub>authors.</sub>


Moststudiescapturedvirtualnessasadichotomousmeasure


(co-located [0] or distributed teams [1]). Only sixteen studies



examined virtualness as a continuous variable (e.g., combined


spatialdistanceandcommunicationmediaasameasure).When


studies captured virtualness as a dichotomous measure, we


transformed thedata to point-biserial correlations which were


used with the correlations for the continuous measures; this


allowedustoexaminetheoveralleffectsofvirtualness.


As with any meta-analysis, we needed to make a series of


coding decisions.Thefirstconcerned studiesinwhich labels of


variables appeared inconsistent with the content of their


measures.Thiswas doneby comparing thelabels in thestudy


withtheitemsusedtooperationalizethem.Atleasttwoauthors


discussed thecontent of a measure and reconciled any


discre-pancieswithitslabel.Thesecondconcernedmoderatorlevels.For


example,regardingteamduration,wehadtodeterminethecut-off


between short- and long-term teams. Moderator levels are



determinedbythedataavailableintheempiricalstudies.Based


onthis,teamswereconsideredshort-termwhentheirmembers


workedtogetherforadayorlessandclassifiedaslong-termwhen


theyworked togetherfor more thana day. Althoughthis time


periodmayseemarbitraryandsmall,wewereconstrainedbythe


availabledata.


Anotherissueconcernedthenecessityofaggregatingnarrow


measures into larger measures. For example, some studies


reported process satisfaction while others reported outcome


satisfaction;wecodedthesedifferentaspectsofsatisfactioninto


thesameoutcomevariablecalled‘‘Satisfaction’’.Wedidthisfor


threevariables:conflict, satisfactionand performance.Tocheck


the appropriateness of aggregation, we ran separate analyses


onthedisaggregatedmeasuresandthencomparedtheconsistency


oftheresults.Forconflict,weconductedseparateanalysesfortask,



relationship,process,andmiscellaneousconflict(suchasoverall


conflict or conflict behaviors); for performance, we conducted


analysesformeasureslabeledasperformanceandmeasureswith


otherperformancelabels(suchas‘creativityofsolution’);andfor


5


Manyofthearticlesdidnotreportallofthedataweneeded.Wecontacted
authorswhoprovidedpartialdata;somewereabletosendusthedata.


6<sub>Some</sub><sub>of</sub><sub>the</sub><sub>studies</sub><sub>reported</sub><sub>mean</sub><sub>differences</sub><sub>(or</sub><sub>some</sub><sub>other</sub><sub>statistic)</sub><sub>on</sub><sub>a</sub>


</div>
<span class='text_page_counter'>(5)</span><div class='page_container' data-page=5>

satisfaction,weconductedanalysesforsatisfactionwithsolution,


satisfaction with process, and other satisfaction (e.g., general


satisfaction).Theresultsshowedthatthedisaggregatedmeasures


hadthesamerelationshipwithvirtualnessastheoverallmeasure,


except for conflict: task conflict was positively related to


virtualness,whiletheotherconflict measureswere


non-signifi-cant.Therefore,taskconflictispresentedseparatelyfromtheother


typesofconflict.



We also made coding decisions for studies that presented


multiplecorrelations forthesame constructs.Because multiple


correlationsinasinglestudycreatethepotentialforoverweighting


studiesthatcontainnon-independentdata,theresultsofa


meta-analysismaybedistorted.Toavoidthis,weusedaconservative


approachofchoosingonecorrelationperstudytominimizethe


problems associated with non-independent coefficients. When


morethanonecorrelationwasreportedforthesameconstructat


thesamepointoftimeinthesamestudy(multipledimensionsof


the same construct), we averaged the correlations into one


correlationinordertocapturethedimensionalityoftheconstruct.


Whenmakingthesedecisions,weusedthedatathatmostclosely


representedteamsinorganizations,makingtheresultspotentially


closer to practice. For example, when data were reported for


multiple points in time, we used the data that most closely



represent actual employee teams (e.g., if correlations were


presentedafter 1 monthand 3 monthsfor thesame variables,


wechosethecorrelationfor3months).Also,iftherewasmore


thanoneface-to-faceconditionintheexperiment(e.g.,bothwith


and without computer support), we chose the condition with


computersupport,asthismostcloselyresemblesactualteamsin


organizations.


5. Analyses


Meta-analysisprovides an estimateofthe‘‘true’’ population


effect size for a given relationship via a series of steps. We


cumulatedcorrelationcoefficientsintoaverageeffectsizes[4,11].


5.1. Computationofeffectsizes


Toexaminethefirstfivehypotheses,weusedsimpleeffectsizes


ofcorrelations7<sub>.</sub><sub>A</sub><sub>number</sub><sub>of</sub><sub>studies</sub><sub>did</sub><sub>not</sub><sub>report</sub><sub>correlations</sub>


but we obtained some either by contacting the authors or by



converting otherreported data (suchas t-tests, F-tests,etc.) to


correlations.


We calculated overall mean effect sizes (r) by weighting


correlationsbysamplesize,correctingboththecorrelationsand


weightings for unreliability, performing Fisher’s


Z-transforma-tions, calculating the overall mean correlations, and then


un-transformingtheoverallmeancorrelations.Theseweretestedfor


significanceusingaz-testandconfidenceintervalsarereported.


(In addition, we report the mean effect sizes (rxy) without


correctingforunreliability).


5.2. Moderatordetectionandestimation


HypothesesH6–H8 wereaddressedbydeterminingwhether


thestudiesincludedin themeta-analysis came from


heteroge-neouspopulations(i.e.,whethermoderatorvariableswerepresent


ornot),and,ifso,whetherthemoderatorvariablesaccountedfora



significantamountoftheresidual variance.The Qstatistic was


used as a test of effect size homogeneity, with a statistically


significantQindicatingheterogeneityofeffectsizes.For


statisti-callysignificantQ’s,weseparatedtheresultsbythemoderator


Table
1
Meta-analyses
of
virtualness
with
processes
and
outcomes.
Team
Inputs
Conflict
(H1)
Communication
frequency
(H2)
Knowledge
sharing
(H3)
Performance
(H4)


Satisfaction
(H5)
k
a
nr
xy
b
r
c(CI)
d
k
a
nr
xy
b
r
c(CI)
d
k
a
nr
xy
b
r
c(CI)
d
k
a
nr
xy

b
r
c(CI)
d
k
a
nr
xy
b
r
c(CI)
d
Degree
of
virtualness
e
(overall)
Task
6
630
0.13
0.14
*
13
2544
0.10
0.11
*
(
0.15, <sub>0.06)</sub>

7
542
0.21
0.22
*
(
0.31, <sub>0.13)</sub>
30
3813
0.09
0.09
*
(
0.12, <sub>0.06)</sub>
23
2717
0.07
0.08
*
(
0.12, <sub>0.04)</sub>
Other
14
908
0.06
0.07
Degree
of


virtualness (categorical measure)



Task
5
471
0.28
0.31
*
9
840
0.06
0.06
3
173
0.60
0.61
*
23
2483
0.15
0.15
*
18
1880
0.15
0.16
*
Other
12
571
0.02


0.02
Degree
of


virtualness (continuous measure)


Task
1
159
0.26
0.33
*
4
1704
0.12
0.14
*
4
369
0.01
0.01
7
1330
0.00
0.00
5
837
0.09
0.11
*


Other
2
337
0.13
0.16
*
a
k
=
number
of
studies.
b
rxy
=
m
ean
weighted
coefficient.
c
r
=
coefficient
corrected
for
the
unreliability
of
predictor
and

criterion.
d
CI:
95%
confidence
interval.
e
A
higher
number
represents
higher
virtualness.
*
z-test:
correlatio
n
is
different
from
0
(p
<
0.05).
7


</div>
<span class='text_page_counter'>(6)</span><div class='page_container' data-page=6>

variables (individuals/groups, experiments/surveys, and short/


long-term)todetermineifthecorrelationsdifferedacrossthese



moderators.


6. Results


Allofourmainandhomogeneityeffectsweresignificant,and


thereforeposthocpoweranalyseswerenotneeded.Nevertheless,


we calculated power and found that it was >0.90 for all


relationshipsexceptforknowledgesharing(power=0.59).Table


1 presents the meta-analyticresults for therelations between


virtualnessandteamprocessesandoutcomes(H1–H5).


We first examined the results for the overall (combined)


measure of virtualness. Hypothesis 1 was supported for task


conflict(r=0.14*),buttheresultswerenotsignificantforother


typesofconflict.Thefindingswereconsistentwiththerestofthe


hypotheses: Hypotheses 2 (r= 0.11*), 3 (r= 0.22*), 4


(r= 0.09*),and5(r= 0.08*)weresupported.


TurningtothetwomeasuresofvirtualnesspresentedinTable1,



fourofthefivehypothesizedrelationshipsdiffersignificantly.For


each of these, the relationships were more likely tobe in the


hypothesizeddirectionforthecategoricalthanforthecontinuous


virtualnessmeasure(seeTable1).


6.1. Moderators


Whenobservingtherelationshipswithintheindividualstudies,


wesawdifferencesbetweenstudies,suggestingthepresenceof


moderators. For team performance, we found 30 studies that


empiricallyexaminedtherelationshipwithvirtualness:sixwith


mediumtorelativelystrongpositiveeffects (definedasarhoof


0.20orlarger),sevenwithmediumtorelativelystrongnegative


effects,and17withamixofweak(positiveornegative)effects.


Studiesalsoshowedconflictingresultsfortheeffectsofvirtualness


on communication frequency, conflict, knowledge sharing, and


satisfaction.



Thepossibilityofmoderators(H6–H8)wasexaminedviatheQ


statistic.AsallhadasignificantQ(p<0.05),wefurtheranalyzed


theserelationshipsbysplittingthedataonthethreemoderatorsof


individual/group, experiment/survey, and short-term/long-term


(seeTable2).Allcomparisonsforlevelofanalysis(groupversus


individual)asa moderator (H6)weresignificant.However, the


relationshipfor satisfaction wasin the opposite directionthan


proposed (with individual level of analysis beingstronger). All


comparisonsfor studymethod(surveyversusexperiment)asa


moderator(H7)weresupported,exceptforotherconflict(withno


significantdifferencefound).Allcomparisonsforteamduration


(short-versuslong-term)weresupported,exceptthat


communi-cationfrequencywasintheoppositedirectionthanthatproposed


(withlong-termbeingmorenegative).


7. Discussionandimplications



Ourfirstgoalforthismeta-analysiswastoexaminetheextent


to which research presents a cumulative body of knowledge


regardinghowvirtualnessaffectsteamfunctioning.Wefoundthis


tobelacking.DrawingonthetraditionalIPOmodel,thetheoretical


perspectivesofmediarichness/socialpresence,attributiontheory,


andcategorizationhelpedtoexplainourhypotheses.Theoverall


results, summarized using80 datasets (representingresponses


from over several thousand participants), supported our main


hypotheses:morevirtualteamsexhibithighertaskconflictand


lower communication frequency, knowledge sharing,


perfor-mance,and satisfaction. Althoughthesefindings are consistent


withmorerecentresearch,ourresultssuggestthattheresultsdo


not generalize to all types of teams and methodological


approaches.


Our choice of moderators was dependent on the empirical



articles available, and we were able to examine only one


theoretically-based moderator, time. We found that higher


virtualness in short-termteams (a dayor less in ouranalysis)


didnegativelyaffecttheteam(e.g.,moreconflict,less


communi-cation and knowledge sharing, weaker performance and lower


satisfaction).However,inlonger-termteams,virtualnessdidnot


havenearlythesamedetrimentaleffects.Therewasnonegative


effectonteamperformanceandsatisfaction,andteamconflictwas


reduced as virtualness increased. While still havinga negative


effectoncommunicationfrequency andknowledgesharing,the


effect wasconsiderably weakerthan in short-term teams.Itis


importanttonote,however,thatmostofthestudiesinvestigating


short-term teams (a day or less) involved students, whereas


studiesfocusingonlonger-termteams(morethanaday)involved


studentsaswellasemployees.



Virtualnessclearlyhasadifferenteffectonteamsdependingon


thelengthoftimetheteamworkstogether.Forshort-termteams,


leaner media, misattributions, and subgroups all potentially


contributeto lesseffective teams.For example,mediarichness


theorywouldsuggestthattheslowerabilitytocommunicaterich


Table2


Hypothesizedmoderators.


Relationships Q Levelofanalysis
(H6)


ra


(kb


,n) Method(H7) ra


(kb


,n) Teamduration
(H8)


ra



(kb


,n)


Virtualness–Taskconflict 77* <sub>Individual</sub>c <sub>0.05</sub><sub>(2,433)</sub> <sub>Experiment</sub>c <sub>0.33</sub>*<sub>(3,404)</sub> <sub>Short</sub>c <sub>0.37</sub>*<sub>(2,355)</sub>


Groupc


0.33*


(4,197) Surveyc


0.20*


(2,226) Longc


0.18*


(4,275)
Virtualness–Otherconflict 59*


Individualc


0.21*


(3,420) Experiment 0.05(10,504) Shortc


0.03(8,372)
Groupc



0.05(11,488) Survey 0.09*


(4,404) Longc


0.16*


(5,358)
Virtualness–Communication


frequency


252*


Individualc


0.04(9,2239) Experimentc


0.01(7,600) Shortb


0.05(6,517)
Groupc


0.30*


(4,305) Surveyc


0.11*


(6,1944) Longc



0.18*


(6,758)
Virtualness–Knowledgesharing 67*


Individualc


0.15*


(3,318) Experimentc


0.61*


(3,173) Shortc


0.67*


(1,59)
Groupc <sub>0.34</sub>*<sub>(4,224)</sub> <sub>Survey</sub>c <sub>0.01</sub><sub>(3,353)</sub> <sub>Long</sub>c <sub>0.10</sub>*<sub>(5,417)</sub>


Virtualness–Performance 276* <sub>Individual</sub>c <sub>0.04</sub><sub>(9,2220)</sub> <sub>Experiment</sub>c <sub>0.17</sub>*<sub>(12,889)</sub> <sub>Short</sub>c <sub>0.27</sub>*<sub>(11,</sub><sub>750)</sub>


Groupc <sub>0.19</sub>*<sub>(21,1593)</sub> <sub>Survey</sub>c <sub>0.08</sub>*<sub>(16,2924)</sub> <sub>Long</sub>c <sub>0.07</sub><sub>(16,1919)</sub>


Virtualness–Satisfaction 262*


Individualc


0.11*



(14,1990) Experimentc


0.03*


(16,1525) Shortc


0.22*


(12,1178)
Groupc


0.00(9,727) Surveyc


0.12*


(7,1192) Longc


0.05(10,1361)


a


r=coefficientcorrectedfortheunreliabilityofpredictorandcriterion.


b


k=numberofstudies.


</div>
<span class='text_page_counter'>(7)</span><div class='page_container' data-page=7>

informationandinteractbackandforthviaelectronicmediawould


havea muchlargerimpactonteameffectivenessforshort-term



teamsthanforteamswithseveraldaysorweekstoaccomplish


their tasks. (However students in large courses mayalso have


manypeopleintheirgroupswhoknowandhaveworkedwithone


another).Forlongertermteams,memberswouldalsobelesslikely


tomakemisattributionstotheperson:asteammembersinteract,


theybuildknowledgeabouteachotheranddeveloprelationships.


Also, as knowledge of the underlying context and the parties


involvedinthecommunicationsgrows,theabilityfor


understand-ingandtherichnessofthemessagesalsogrow.Similarly,aspeople


start to identify with a team and build a team identity and


psychologicalandaffectivetiestotheteam,thishelpstoovercome


othersourcesofdifferences.


Intermsofmethodsvariables,wefounddifferingresultsbased


on several comparisons.First, resultsaremore encouragingfor


studies using continuous rather than categorical measures of



virtualness. That is, for studies with continuous measures of


virtualness,therelationshipwithtaskconflict ismore negative


(lower conflict for more virtual teams), the relationships with


knowledgesharingandsatisfactionaremorepositive,andthereis


noimpactonperformance.Measuringvirtualnessasacategorical


variablemightbeasimplisticviewthatdoesnotcapturethereality


of virtual work in a natural setting. It might be portraying a


‘‘spurious’’ view of the effects of virtualness on processes and


outcomesintherealworld.Studiesusingacategoricalmeasureare


morelikelytobeexperimentswithstudentsandexperimentsare


designedusingstrongermanipulations,resultinginbiggereffect


sizes.Becausestudiesusingcontinuousmeasuresaremorelikely


to investigate longer-term employee teams using a survey


methodology,wemightbeabletobemoreoptimisticconcerning


virtual team functioning in organizations. Furthermore, past



researchon virtualteamshasoftenconfoundedtechnology use


withmeasuresofvirtualness.Thatis,theycompareddistributed


groupsusingonetool(suchase-mail)withco-locatedgroupswith


no tools. Further, for the articles that provided bundles of


technologies to team members, there was no single set of


technologiesprovidedacrossthearticlesandthereforewecould


not determine the effects of particular technologies for virtual


teams.


Wefoundthatanothermethodsvariable,thelevelofanalysis


(groupversusindividual)alsomoderatedthevirtualness


relation-ships.Asexpected,therewerestrongereffectsofvirtualnesson


taskconflict,communicationfrequency,knowledgesharing,and


performance at the group level than at the individual level.


However, the effects of virtualness on other conflict and


satisfaction were stronger at the individual level than at the



grouplevel.


7.1. Implicationsforresearchandpractice


Researchers shouldbevery specific aboutthe natureof the


teamstheyarestudying(e.g.,definehowlongtheyhaveexisted)


and use theoretical arguments and empirical findings that


appropriately match the types of teams. Our results provide


valuable advice for designers and leaders of virtualteams. For


practitioners, ourresultssuggested that virtual teamprocesses


wouldbemorenegativeintheshort-term.Organizationswillneed


toexpectthesedifficultiesand putintoplacestructurestohelp


minimizetheirimpacts.Recognizingthatvirtualteamswillhave


communication and conflict challenges (which influence team


functioning),weneedtohelpidentifywaystoimprove


effective-ness. For example,team membersshould betrained tohandle


conflicttominimizeitsnegativeimpactonproductivity.For



long-termteams,practitionersshouldexpectinitialchallenges,butthey


couldmovemorerapidlytolessnegativestages.


7.2. Limitations


Aswithanyresearch,this meta-analysishaslimitations that


mustbeconsideredwheninterpretingtheresults.Meta-analytical


workisonlyasmeaningfulastheprimarystudiesfromwhichitis


derived.Wefoundalackofconsistencyconcerningmeasuresand


reportingstandards.Becauseofthelargevarietyofmeasuresused,


we sometimes had to collapse narrow measures into larger


categories, reducing the variance in some measures. A further


weakness in the literature is the small number of studies


examiningsomerelationships.Althoughwesearchedfor


unpub-lished papers, this study is still subject to the so-called ‘‘file


drawer’’problem,orthedifficultyoffindingnon-significantresults


sincetheytendtostayunpublished.Wealsofoundarticlesthatdid



notreportneededinformationsuchasthelevelofanalysis.More


disturbingwasthelackofreporteddataneededtoconductthe


analyses(effectsizes,samplesizes,etc.):wehadtoexcludemany


papersbecauseofalackofnecessarydata(whoseauthorscould


notprovideit).


Itisalsoimportanttonotethatmoderatorsoftencanrelateto


each other(e.g., in our reviewedstudies, we found that study


method[experiment/survey]correlated0.68withtime duration


[short/long]).Fewstudiestrytoseparatetheirindependenteffects.


Althoughweexaminedtheseeffectswithourmoderatoranalyses,


itisnotclearwhetherresultsforstudiesusingcontinuousvirtual


measuresaremoreencouraging,becausethemethodtendstobe


non-experimentalandtheseteamshaveaccesstoawiderrangeof


communication tools, and because teams are made up of


employees,orarelonger-term.



8. Conclusion


Ourmeta-analyticreviewdemonstratedthatnotallvirtualness


measuresarethesameandthatsomeconflictingresultsaredueto


thisandothermoderatorvariables(suchasstudymethod).Asa


result,thisstudyhighlightsandhelpsexplainconflictingresults,


andpointstoempiricalgapsinourknowledgeandthenecessityto


informpracticebybuildingacumulativebodyofresearchinwhich


variablesaresystematicallyinvestigated.


Theresultsofourmeta-analysissuggestthatteamprocesses


andoutputsarenegativelyinfluencedbyvirtualness.However,


theseresultsdonotgeneralizetoalltypesofteamsandanalytical


approaches.Themoderationanalysisindicatesthatvirtualness


hasadifferentinfluenceonteamfunctioningdependingonhow


itismeasuredaswellasonthelengthoftimetheteamworks


together.Thenegativeeffectsfoundinshort-termteamsweaken



or disappear in teams that havelonger lives. Ourresults also


pointtoimportant differencesontheeffectsof virtualnessfor


differentlevelsof analysis(individual vs.group)andmethods


(experiments vs.surveys). Weneedto know more about how


differentlevelsofanalysisinteractwitheachother,aswell as


utilizefinerandmultidimensionalmeasuresofvirtualness.Allof


thiscanhelpusunderstandhowtoimproveteamprocessesand


outcomestomorefullytakeadvantageofthepotentialbenefits


ofvirtualwork.


Acknowledgements


We would like to thank Derek Chapman, Peter Dacin, and


SandyHershcovisfortheiradviceonthemeta-analyticanalyses.


An earlier version was presented at the Symposium on High


PerformanceProfessionalTeams,Queen’sUniversity,Canada.This


paperwasfinanciallyassistedbytheIndustrialRelationsCentre,



Queen’s University, and by Social Sciences and Humanities


Research Councilof Canada grants to Sandy Staplesand Jane


</div>
<span class='text_page_counter'>(8)</span><div class='page_container' data-page=8>

References


[1]B.Alge,C.Wiethoff,H.J.Klein,Whendoesthemediummatter?
Knowledge-buildingexperiencesandopportunitiesindecision-makingteamsOrganizational
BehaviorandHumanDecisionProcesses9,2003,pp.26–37.


[2]D.J.Beal,R.R.Cohen,M.J.Burke,C.L.McLendon,Cohesionandperformancein
groups:ameta-analyticclarificationofconstructrelations,JournalofApplied
Psychology88,2003,pp.989–1004.


[3]S.T.Bell,Deep-levelcompositionvariablesaspredictorsofteamperformance:a
meta-analysis,JournalofAppliedPsychology92,2007,pp.595–615.
[4]M.Borenstein,L.V.Hedges,J.P.T.Higgins,H.R.Rothstein,Introductionto


Meta-Analysis, Wiley,Chichester,England,2009.


[5]T.Carte,L.Chidambaram,Acapabilities-basedtheoryoftechnologydeployment
indiverseteams:leapfroggingthepitfallsofdiversityandleveragingitspotential
withcollaborativetechnology,JournaloftheAssociationforInformationSystems
5,2004,pp.448–471.


[6]C.D.Cramton,K.L.Orvis,J.M.Wilson,Situationinvisibilityandattributionin
distributedcollaborations,JournalofManagement33,2007,pp.525–546.
[7]C.D.Cramton, S.S.Webber,Relationshipsamonggeographicdispersion,team



processes,andeffectivenessinsoftwaredevelopmentworkteams,Journalof
BusinessResearch58,2005,pp.758–765.


[8]C.K.W.DeDreu,L.R.Weingart,Taskversusrelationshipconflict:team
perfor-mance, and team membersatisfaction: ameta-analysis, JournalofApplied
Psychology88,2003,pp.741–749.


[9]P.J.Hinds,R.Bailey,Outofsight,outofsync:understandingconflictindistributed
teams,OrganizationScience14,2003,pp.615–632.


[10]C.Lin,C.Standing,Y.C.Liu,Amodeltodevelopeffectivevirtualteams,Decision
SupportSystems45,2008,pp.1031–1045.


[11]M.W.Lipsey,D.B.Wilson,PracticalMeta-Analysis, SagePublications,Thousand
Oaks,CA,2001.


[12]L.L.Martins,L.L.Gilson,M.T.Maynard,Virtualteams:whatdoweknowand
wheredowegofromhere?JournalofManagement30,2004,pp.805–835.
[13]A.P. Massey,M. Montoya-Weiss,M. Hung,Because time matters:temporal


coordinationinglobalvirtualprojectteams,JournalofManagementInformation
Systems19,2003,pp.129–156.


[14]J.Mathieu,M.T.Maynard,T.Rapp,L.Gilson,Teameffectiveness1997–2007:a
reviewofrecentadvancesandaglimpseintothefuture,JournalofManagement
34,2008,pp.410–476.


[15]M.T.Maynard,J.E.Mathieu,T.L.Rapp,L.L.Gilson,Something(s)oldand
some-thing(s)new:modelingdriversofglobalvirtualteameffectiveness,Journalof
OrganizationalBehavior33,2012,pp.342–365.



[16]J.R. Mesmer-Magnus,L.A. DeChurch,M. Jimenez-Rodriguez,J. Wildman, M.
Shuffler,Ameta-analyticinvestigationofvirtualityandinformationsharingin
tea,OrganizationalBehaviorandHumanDecisionProcesses115,2011,pp.214–
225.


[17]M.O’Leary,J.N.Cummings,Thespatial,temporal,andconfigurational
character-isticsofgeographicdispersioninteams,MISQuarterly31,2007,pp.433–452.
[18]N.R.Quigley,P.E.Tesluk,E.A.Locke,K.M.Bartol,Amultilevelinvestigationofthe


motivationalmechanismsunderlyingknowledgesharingandperformance,
Or-ganizationalScience18,2007,pp.71–88.


[19]L.Schweitzer,L.Duxbury,Conceptualizingandmeasuringthevirtualityofteams,
InformationSystemsJournal20,2010,pp.267–295.


[20]S.M.B.Thatcher,K.A.Jehn,E.Zanutto,Cracksindiversityresearch:theeffectsof
diversityfaultlinesonconflictandperformance,GroupDecisionandNegotiation
12,2003,pp.217–241.


[21]R.L.Wakefield,D.E.Leidner,G.Garrison,Amodelofconflict,leadership,and
performanceinvirtualteams,InformationSystemsResearch19,2008,pp.434–
455.


Ana Ortiz de Guinea is an assistant professor of
InformationSystemsatHECMontre´al.SheholdsaPhD
inInformationSystemsfromQueen’sUniversity,aMSc
fromthe University ofLethbridge, and adegree in
ComputerScienceandEngineeringfromthe
Universi-daddeDeusto. Prior toreturning to academia,she


workedas an Information Systemsconsultant. Her
researchhasbeenpublishedinComputersinHuman
Behavior,theInternationalJournalofHumanResource
Management,theInternationalJournalofe-Collaboration,
theJournalofGlobalInformationManagement,andMIS
Quarterly.


Jane Webster received her PhD from New York
University and is the E. Marie Shantz Professor of
MISintheSchoolofBusinessatQueen’sUniversityin
Canada.She has served asa Senior Editorfor MIS
Quarterly,VPofPublicationsforAIS,andadvisorfor
AIS-SIGCHI.She haspublished in a variety of journals
includingtheAcademyofManagementJournal,
Commu-nicationResearch,InformationandOrganization,
Infor-mationSystemsJournal,InformationSystemsResearch,
JournalofOrganizationalBehavior,MISQuarterly,and
OrganizationScience. Her current research concerns
information systems and technologies to support
environmentalsustainability


</div>

<!--links-->
<a href=' />

×